乌鸦比我们想象的还要聪明
Crows are even smarter than we thought

原始链接: https://nautil.us/crows-are-even-smarter-than-we-thought-820066/

由俄罗斯和英国研究人员组成的团队发现,头巾乌鸦具有记住物品外观并重新创造它的认知能力,这表明它们可以形成物品的“心理模板”。 这些心理表征使他们能够生产工具,提高生存机会。 这种技能可能起源于所有鸦科动物的共同祖先,甚至延伸到鸟类世界之外。 这项研究建立在之前对一种名叫贝蒂的新喀里多尼亚乌鸦的研究结果之上,这种乌鸦无需明确教导即可制造工具。 这意味着通过观察或经验创建心理模板的能力可能比之前认为的鸦科动物和其他动物物种更为普遍。

头巾乌鸦表现出与新喀里多尼亚乌鸦相似的行为,新喀里多尼亚乌鸦以其工具使用能力而闻名。 这些行为包括用新材料制造工具、为特定任务选择合适的工具以及从以前的经验中学习。 此前,这种行为被认为是新喀里多尼亚乌鸦所独有的。 研究人员认为,这些能力在乌鸦家族中的传播范围可能比最初想象的更广泛。 此外,研究还提到头巾乌鸦的对应物戈芬凤头鹦鹉也表现出类似的工具使用行为,进一步支持了这一假设。 虽然媒体将这一发现称为乌鸦与灵长类动物一样聪明的启示,但研究人员表示,重点应该放在更广泛的发现上,即这种行为不仅存在于新喀里多尼亚乌鸦身上。 文章提到了印度北部村庄中有关乌鸦的各种文化信仰,乌鸦与客人的到来、在季风季节喂养祖先以及象征已故亲人的来访有关。 作者分享了观察乌鸦习惯的个人轶事,指出它们在盗窃食物(尤其是冬季黄油)方面的有效性,以及它们软化树枝以方便筑巢的策略。 最后,作者讨论了将这些观察结果传达给没有失语症的个体所面临的挑战,失语症涉及形成心理图像的困难。 尽管面临这一挑战,作者仍继续根据他的记忆进行绘画,并根据需要填补空白。 作者承认,虽然艺术性可能并不特殊,但在没有基于图像的内部表示的情况下创建图像的挑战可能会成为障碍。
相关文章

原文

Crows and ravens, which belong to the corvid family, are known for their high intelligence, playful natures, and strong personalities. They hold grudges against each other, do basic statistics, perform acrobatics, and even host funerals for deceased family members. But we keep learning new things about the savvy of these birds, and how widespread that savvy is among the corvid family.

Earlier this year, a team of researchers from Lomonosov Moscow State University in Russia and the University of Bristol found that a species of crow called the hooded crow—which has a gray bust and black tail and head feathers, making it look like it is wearing a “hood”—is able to manage a mental feat we once thought was unique to humans: to memorize the shape and size of an object after it is taken away—in this case a small piece of colored paper—and to reproduce one like it.

This kind of feat, according to animal behavior researchers, requires the ability to form “mental templates.” Essentially, a mental template is an image in the mind of what a particular object looks like, even when that object is not present. Mental templates allow animals to create tools, which can be used to get food or make a stronger nest, both ultimately leading to a better chance of survival. They might also make it possible for individuals to learn about tool making from other members of their species—and to pass along improvements in tool making over time, often called “cumulative culture,” which so far seems rare among non-human animals.

Young crows learn how to make different types of tools by stealing their parent’s tools.

We have been looking for evidence that different corvid and other bird species can create mental templates since at least 2002. That year, researchers published findings showing that Betty, a captive New Caledonian crow, was able to spontaneously bend a piece of wire to create a hook that she could use to grab a hard-to-reach treat. Betty had successfully used a pre-made hook to obtain the treat in earlier trials but in follow-up tasks didn’t seem to fully understand how hooks work.  The researchers decided she must have formed a mental template of the hook, which she then reproduced. So far, researchers have found that Goffin cockatoos, a kind of parrot, can also create tools spontaneously, which could indicate similar mental agility.

But the new hooded crow findings suggest that the ability to learn this way could be more widespread than we thought, says Sarah Jelbert, a comparative psychologist who studies animal behavior at the University of Bristol and is one of the authors of the study. Creating and using mental templates might be a skill that evolved in the ancestor of all corvids, the “Corvida” branch of songbirds, or perhaps it is even shared more broadly across the animal kingdom, she says.

For their study, Jelbert and her colleagues first trained three hooded crows—Glaz (15 years old), Rodya (4 years old), and Joe (3 years old)—to recognize pieces of paper of different sizes and colors. To do this, they exposed the birds to “template” pieces of paper in different colors and sizes for several minutes before removing them—and then rewarded the birds for dropping scraps that matched these templates into a small slit.

In Body Image
BIRD BRAINS: A New Caledonian crow creating a scrap of colored paper that matches the template with which it was trained. Photo by Sarah Jelbert, University of Bristol.

The crows were next given the opportunity to manufacture versions of these objects in exchange for a reward. The researchers found that all three crows manufactured objects that matched the original template object they had been rewarded for in both color and size—even though the treats in this second stage of the experiment were awarded at random. The researchers also observed that Glaz, the oldest of the three hooded crows, seemed to be the most proficient at making scraps that looked like the ones the bird was trained on. This finding suggested to them that mental templates may be linked to experience garnered with age.

“Unlike humans, who regularly copy each other’s behavior … we don’t have much evidence that crows will watch each other and deliberately copy what another crow is doing,” Jelbert says. However, they will steal each other’s tools—in particular, juvenile crows often steal their parents’ tools when they are young. So it’s possible that young crows learn how to make different types of tools from experience stealing their parent’s tools, using them, remembering what these tools look like, and then trying to create something similar, Jelbert says.

What qualifies as a mental template, and how flexible these templates are, seems to be up for some debate. Research suggests birdsong and mating practices may rely on certain kinds of mental templates, which can backfire if a bird memorizes behavior from the wrong species. “For example, if a song sparrow gets imprinted on the song of a swamp sparrow and sings a song from a different species rather than its own, it will have difficulty finding mating partners,” explains Andreas Nieder, a professor of animal psychology at the University of Tübingen and a lead researcher on corvid neuroscience, who was not involved in this study. “Similarly, if one finch species gets sexually imprinted on another, it may show courtship displays to the wrong species in adulthood.”

Nieder says this kind of imprinting can become fixed in the bird’s brain, and is not changeable even in new environments. “In this case, templates may no longer represent intelligence but rather the opposite,” he adds. Researchers have not yet determined whether mental templates related to tool making remain flexible, though there is some evidence in New Caledonian crows that they may evolve.

For biologists and comparative psychologists, understanding the ways corvids use mental templates can help to illuminate not just the nature of bird intelligence, but of intelligence across the animal kingdom and evolutionary time.

Lead image: RuqayaMai / Shutterstock

联系我们 contact @ memedata.com